A Dive Into Homeostasis

Homeostasis is the subject, and under review here is an enormous piece of work, published by Frontiers in Physiology by George E. Billman. He credits pioneers Claude Bernard and Walter Cannon with spreading the notion that “the health and vitality of the organism can be said to be the end result of homeostatic regulation,” and also the concept that a physician’s role is to “clear the path so that nature could take its course.”

Billman wrote,

[H]omeostatic regulation is not merely the product of a single negative feedback cycle but reflects the complex interaction of multiple feedback systems that can be modified by higher control centers. This hierarchical control and feedback redundancy results in a finer level of control and a greater flexibility that enables the organism to adapt to changing environmental conditions.

Regulation is an interesting word, because rather than rolling up on a problem with heavy artillery, it suggests more of an incremental approach. One regulates a thermostat, or a carburetor. In order for this to be effective, the parts and the system have to work together as a complex and multifactorial community.

Homeostasis, then, is the tendency of a system to maintain an internal stability as the result of the coordinated response of its parts to any situation or stimulus that disturbs normal conditions or function.

As previously noted, Cannon specified that homeostasis does not mean immobility, but “a condition which may vary, but is relatively constant.” Various things are going on at three different levels:

— physiochemical processes, the organ and tissue functions, the component parts upon which homeostasis acts.
— autonomous (self) regulation… [C]hanges in a given variable are sensed and adjustments of the first level processes are initiated…
— central command and control centers (central nervous system) that process the information transmitted from the second level and integrates it with information from other sensory inputs to coordinate the physiological and behavioral response to changing environmental conditions.

The higher centers can intervene either consciously or unconsciously, but only — and this is vital — if the first and second levels are working properly. For good or ill, there are also higher levels of control outside the organism. The body can signal urgently that it needs water, and the conscious mind may concur, but the reality of the situation, e.g. being lost in a desert, will win.

Another outside control mechanism might be a medical care system that cannot or will not take the appropriate measures. In his Summary, Billman reaffirms that disease is caused by disruption of the homeostatic mechanisms, and “effective therapy must be directed toward re-establishing these homeostatic conditions, working with rather than against nature.”

J. S. Turner in 2017 suggested “dynamic disequilibrium” as a fitting description of homeostasis, and went so far as to say “homeostasis is life’s fundamental property, what distinguishes it from non-life. In short, homeostasis is life.”

Your responses and feedback are welcome!

Source: “Homeostasis: The Underappreciated and Far Too Often Ignored Central Organizing Principle of Physiology,” Frontiersin.org/articles, 03/10/20
Image by Pat Hartman

Homeostasis — a Word and a Paradox

Along with the internet came the ability of anyone to invent or define a word, and pretty much make it stick. In order to see what the original behavioralists were talking about, we go back in time to 1900, when Charles R. Richet wrote,

The living system is stable… [I]t must be in order not to be destroyed, dissolved or disintegrated by colossal forces, often adverse, which surround it. By an apparent contradiction, it maintains its stability only if it is excitable and capable of modifying itself according to external stimuli and adjusting its response to the stimulation. In a sense, it is stable because it is modifiable — the slight instability is the necessary condition for the true stability of the organism.

To express this concept of an organism in constant interaction with a world in which change might be the only constant, Walter Cannon coined the word homeostasis. The root words were Greek for “similar” and “standing still,” and what he meant was not staying the same, but staying similar. Since then, some have misread homeostasis to mean unchanging or even stagnant, but no. It describes the self-regulating processes a biological system uses to maintain stability, adjusting all the while to changing environmental conditions.

A balancing act

In a very recent and extremely thorough exploration of the basic concept of homeostasis, George E. Billman (of Ohio State University’s Department of Physiology and Cell Biology) says it is “the complex interaction of multiple feedback systems that can be modified by higher control centers,” and that homeostasis has become…

[…] the central unifying concept of physiology and is defined as a self-regulating process by which an organism can maintain internal stability while adjusting to changing external conditions.

He goes into more detail and reaffirms Cannon’s thoughts:

[…] This hierarchical control and feedback redundancy results in a finer level of control and a greater flexibility that enables the organism to adapt to changing environmental conditions.

[…] Homeostasis is not static and unvarying; it is a dynamic process that can change internal conditions as required to survive external challenges.

So, we are talking about two things: internal stability within a range of values (like the vital signs of temperature, pulse rate and blood pressure being “within normal limits”) and the self-regulatory goal-seeking behavior known as the “coordinated dynamic response that maintains this internal stability.” Billman expresses the wish to see greater emphasis placed on traditional integrated, holistic approaches to healing, and closer adherence to the ideas of those who “gave birth to physiology as an intellectual discipline.” He writes,

The disruption of homeostatic mechanisms is what leads to disease, and effective therapy must be directed toward re-establishing these homeostatic conditions, working with rather than against nature.

Everybody, and literally every body, is out there trying to maintain balance while satisfying their drives and avoiding a terrible fall. The photo on this page, where two people are on a tightrope at the same time, is illustrative of society. Each person strives to maximize their own wins and minimize their own losses, while not allowing someone else’s mistakes to throw them off the wire.

Your responses and feedback are welcome!

Source: “Homeostasis: The Underappreciated and Far Too Often Ignored Central Organizing Principle of Physiology,” Frontiersin.org/articles, 03/10/20
Image by wht_wolf9653/CC BY-SA 2.0

It’s Complicated, Continued

Scholars tend to describe drive theory as having been very important and influential, back when it formed the thinking of current experts. In any field, pioneers have the opportunity to pursue false notions and follow unproductive trails, so that later practitioners can stand on their shoulders.

In 1992, D. Maestripieri and three co-authors published an article that reawakened interest in the early animal behaviorists so that the contemporary ones started to pay attention again. They were interested in the idea of displacement behavior as “a good measure of anxiety levels, ” wrote Juan D. Delius, and…

Since then a sizeable literature evaluating the effectiveness of displacement behavior measures (also called self directed behavior, or SDB) as indicators of anxiety has grown.

On the other hand, many of the earliest writers’ thoughts have “fallen out of favor,” and their work receives respect but not devotion:

Drive-reduction theory is most widely criticized or disregarded by contemporary psychologists because it fails to adequately account for behaviors that are outside the purview of strictly physiological needs like thirst or hunger and behaviors that involve complex external factors.

One really serious wrinkle

It seems like any meaningful research would be difficult to undertake, in the whole topic, because of the human propensity to cultivate mild preferences into drives, and to invent drives out of thin air. Humans synthesize drugs that both create and satisfy our drive to snort them. And, the serious drugs of abuse actually replace innate drives. Given the choice between heroin and food, a committed user will take the heroin. So, what does that do to the meaning of a word like “innate”?

A physiological need is one that an individual or a species could die from the absence of. Granted, people die from heroin overdose — from an artificially induced need; from a false drive. Only the damage is real. At any rate, this is a serious problem, but not a species-killer.

In other examples, humans don’t wait around until natural impulse moves them to want sex. A vast pornography industry exists to intentionally create and stimulate that particular drive. The urge to sneeze is an irresistible physiological force. Throughout most of the world, in various eras, snuff has been used to purposefully induce sneezes, just for fun. Something more is going on here, than the simple wish to return to peaceful homeostasis.

The multifaceted world

Speaking of different views, drive theory (or drive-reduction theory) accepts and glorifies drives as the only possible influencers of human behavior; and the satisfaction of drives as our purpose in life. At the same time, an entire ancient school of thought is based on the idea that innate, hard-wired, instinctual, automatic, mechanistic behavior is the very thing the human race needs to eradicate if we are ever to become our best selves. Ironically, the ancient techniques for the elimination of drives involve immobile sitting, without even a screen to look at.

Your responses and feedback are welcome!

Source: “Displacement activities and arousal,” Baillement.com, 07/08/03
Source: “AP Psychology Study Resource: Drive Theory,” APPsychology.com, undated
Image by World’s Direction/Public Domain

Drive-Reduction Theory — It’s Complicated

In the area of drive-reduction theory, people seem to have different ideas about definitions.

One source says,

Understand — “physiological need” means something without which you, or the species will die. So it’s a pretty short list — food, water, temperature regulation, sleep, air, and sex. Essentially, this theory says that all human behavior connects to getting one of those needs met.

So apparently, drives are in everybody, all the time. That in itself is a bit difficult to deal with, because a study with no control group has a problem. Instructure.com says a drive is a “motivation, desire or interest to behave or act in a certain manner, often to meet a need.” This might be an unworkably broad definition, as it seems to include everything in life.

An uncredited writer at CommunicationTheory.org says,

Throughout the decades, the Drive Theory has gone through various changes. It has also garnered a lot of critique… “Drive” is an “appetitive internal force”…

[I]n his later works, Freud reduced the emphasis on drives. He said that since the nature of drives is changeable, other factors such as social situations had an important role to play as well.

According to one authority on drive-reduction theory, the term refers to “a diverse set of motivational theories in psychology,” which does seem to be the case.

A primary drive is innate and inextricably hooked into the body. A secondary drive, for something like excessive wealth accumulation, is the kind that can be planted in someone’s head, and it seems as if there should be a whole different set of rules. It might be fair to say that a secondary drive is one that can be addressed by therapy, whereas no amount of therapy would stop the need for water, and that pretty much defines a primary drive.

Dr. Pretlow says,

Displacement activity is rechanneling of overflow energy from conflicted or thwarted drives into another drive…

Displacement activity permits the resolution of conflict between two antagonistic drives by acting as an outlet through which overflow energy can be discharged…

Displacement activity is an innate, hard-wired, instinctual, automatic biobehavioral mechanism…

Displacement activity stems from situations of major opposing or thwarted behavioral drives, e.g. fight or flight…

On that last point, some authorities identify additional reactions that rank right up there with fight or flight. If so, that would make things very complicated. Also, it seems as if not everyone is on the same page when it comes to the difference between a drive and the means of satisfying a drive. If the meta-drive is to reduce tension and return to a neutral state of homeostasis, then it seems like a lot of responses, other than fight or flight, could qualify as means to reduce the drive.

(To be continued…)

Your responses and feedback are welcome!

Source: “Drive-Reduction,” Instructure.com, undated
Source: “Drive Theory,” CommunicationTheory.org, undated
Image by Doug Kerr/CC BY-SA 2.0

Furry and Feathered Kids

To think of displacement activities as symptomatic of inner turmoil is useful in two ways. First, in a lab or study setting, or even in a zoo, seeing what they call displacement behavior can give a signal to the humans in charge. Maybe lab animals or zoo animals are being treated more harshly than they need to be.

Pets are a much larger area of interest, personified by Amy Martin of Conscious Companion. As observant pet owners know, an action that is out of character can be a red flag. If a pet does something weird, it might signal an issue that could develop into a significant problem. Why is our awareness of these behaviors important? Martin writes,

Inner conflict that’s not positively addressed can lead to more severe anxiety, fears, and prolonged stress. These can in turn affect an animal’s mental and physical well being, which can lead to medical and behavioral issues.

Martin says that some activities…

[…] have become transformed into signals which convey the frame of mind of one individual to another of the same species.

Fair enough. And equally important, a pet’s frame of mind can be conveyed to individuals of a different species — namely, the humans upon whom they depend for their survival. Martin gives examples of what to look out for. In cats and dogs, displacement behavior can include:

— Yawning when not sleepy
— Grooming out of context
— Shaking off when not wet
— Stretching deeply
— Scent marking with their face

For cats, of course, there is the scratching post, which we purposely provide for them in the hope that it will displace their desire to scratch furniture, or us. It is noteworthy when a cat uses the scratching post after a stressful encounter. Every animal has its own signifiers. When a parrot feels conflicted, Martin cites “beak wiping and scratching” as common displacement behaviors.

What to do?

Martin suggests keeping it “upbeat and easy,” turning the conflict into fun…

[…] or at the very least, help the animal to feel calm, relaxed, and safe. Help them walk away from what’s stressing them, or let them know they are safe by removing the perceived threat. If the situation is getting tense with another animal or child, intervene swiftly but positively. Then offer everyone something positive and productive to focus on.

Dr. Pretlow has written about the similarities shared by obese pets and obese children:

The pet-parent may need to confront and be treated for her/his own addictive eating to cease enabling it in the pet. The pet-parent would need to implement “tough love” and tolerate “cold shoulder” and actual hostility from the pet when reducing treats/food, as well as seek alternative sources of companionship.

Your responses and feedback are welcome!

Source: “Doin’ the Displacement,” ConsciousCompanion2012.com, August 2015
Source: “Similarities between obesity in pets and children: the addiction model,” Cambridge.org, 07/29/16
Image by Cynthia Donovan/CC BY 2.0

Do Animals Have Things Figured Out? Continued

The topic continues to be thatch ants who, as we have seen, build enormous domiciles for themselves and their friends. The top part of the abode is a mound secured with dried plant matter, and the living quarters extend into underground tunnels and rooms. All the babies are kept in the central brood chamber. The sun warms the upper part of the home, while underneath, the decay of organic material generates heat. The ants forage for food at ground level, and also high up in trees.

If nearby plants try to encroach and grab too much territory, the ants poison them. An impressive thatch ant colony in Oregon consisted of 210 nests, covering four hectares (almost 10 acres or 48,000 square yards) of ground, and inhabited by probably 56 million individuals. Under normal circumstances, the acute, immediate stress of a predatory incursion rouses the worker ants to mount a vigorous defense of the colony. Against bird, bear, or whoever bothers them, they stand their ground.

If humans poke around, the ants will directly attack:

When a colony was disturbed the surface of the mound was soon covered with workers. Many assumed the defensive position: head up and mandibles widely spread; gaster turned forward under the thorax and ready to spray formic acid into any wound made by the mandibles. Many workers started spraying at the beginning of the disturbance and soon there was an invisible cloud of formic acid vapor above the nest that was irritating to human eyes and noses. The bites of the workers were also annoying.

But priorities can change. Three months after a wildfire had devastated an area, researchers studied thatch ant colonies that had suffered the loss of food sources and beneficent foliage cover, along with massive damage to the aboveground parts of their dwellings:

Many ants were directly killed by the fire […] but some ants survived in underground portions of the nest. We show that the behavioral fight-or-flight response of ants is altered…

After the fire, and after many weeks of living in an atmosphere of chronic stress, the ants manifested an altered response to events that caused acute stress. They became much more likely to flee from aggression, and much less inclined to fight. Their reaction proposes a big, intriguing question.

Could this response also serve a positive purpose? After all, thatch ants have elegantly dealt with so many other issues. Maybe the apparent discouragement is a necessary phase that gives the community a chance to pause, process the current reality, and form a consensus for change. Maybe this is nature’s way of telling them to get out of there and establish a new colony. Basically, we don’t really know what’s going on with them, except that they have been working on their game plans for millennia.

Are animals competent?

An animal will sometimes respond to a threat by doing essentially nothing; grass-picking or some other action that humans call displacement behavior, and regard as a substitute for action they consider more correct. Human observers give the animals a failing grade because they don’t engage in combat or run away, or whatever we have decided they should properly do.

Can we justify labeling the actions as inappropriate, when it might be that we simply don’t understand? In the face of horrendous threats, many animals comport themselves quite capably. Maybe they know exactly what they are doing when they pick at grass, too, or engage in some other action that seems random, out of context or incorrect to us. As oblivious bipeds who don’t know enough to flee from a tsunami, are we really authorized to make rulings about acceptable, effective threat responses?

Everything that behavioralists believe about human drives, and the human tendency to practice displacement activities, may be perfectly valid. Still, it seems rather unfair to take these judgment errors and lay them at the door of animals, who generally seem to have a pretty good handle on things.

Your responses and feedback are welcome!

Source: “Formica obscuripes,” AntWiki.org, undated
Source: “Climate change and wildfire-induced alteration of fight-or-flight behavior,” ScienceDirect.com, July 2021
Image by Upupa4me/CC BY-SA 2.0

Do Animals Have Things Figured Out?

Various creatures, confronted with approaching natural disaster, are able to satisfy their basic, innate drive to survive such catastrophes. Many species have developed effective ways to deal with hurricanes, floods, wildfires, and tornadoes. Wolf experts say,

When wolves first smell smoke, they’ll gather together. They’ll howl for each other to locate every member of the pack, and then they’ll stay close to each other in order to protect each other. They’ll patrol their territory to figure out where the threat of fire is coming from.

In the land bordering the Indian Ocean, prior to an underwater earthquake, elephants and flamingos fled for higher ground. In Thailand, a herd of buffalo near the beach pricked up their ears, looked out to sea, and then stampeded to the top of a hill. Cows, goats, cats, and birds also knew, somehow, to move inland.

Even in 373 BC, Thucydides reported that rats, dogs, weasels, and snakes deserted the city of Helice days before a huge earthquake. In 1805 in Italy, sheep, dogs and geese knew something was up, shortly pre-earthquake. Toads deserted their mating grounds. In Sicily, goats knew to flee the area in advance of a violent volcanic eruption, and just before the 1906 San Francisco quake, horses ran away.

In China, a quake alert system depends on snakes, who will move out of their nests even when the cold would normally encourage them to stay in their cozy homes. In 2014, birds in the Cumberland Mountains of Tennessee abandoned their breeding ground just before 80 tornadoes hit the area. A large French project studies migratory birds who avoid storms over the Pacific.

Even bugs have agendas

When a fire sweeps through an area, some insects rush to arrive on the scene while the trees are still actively burning. Are they nuts, reacting to a threat in such an inappropriate, out-of-contest manner? Absolutely not, because the best place to lay their eggs is in freshly burned wood, so they exploit and capitalize on the devastation by crowding out competitors for the enviable nursery spots, which works out splendidly for them.

Consider the lowly thatch ant (or thatching ant), Formica obscuripes. When starting a new colony, they will claim an open area of ground and establish, under a piece of wood or a stone, a nest that then grows both upward and downward. They build a mound from plant debris, and are very adaptable in the materials they use and how big they decide to make the mounds. They are also gangster enough to take over others’ territory:

These ants are temporary social parasites of Formica fusca-group species, including Formica pacifica. New colonies are established when an inseminated F. obscuripes queen enters the nest of a Formica fusca-group species and is accepted by the host workers. The host queen is eventually killed or driven off, and the host workers raise the brood of the invading queen. Eventually, only F. obscuripes workers remain as the original host workers die off.

But rather than repelling all visitors, they will also, for reasons best known to themselves, accept different kinds of insects as housemates — probably because the guests perform useful functions for the community that humans do not completely understand.

(To be continued…)

Your responses and feedback are welcome!

Source: “What does wildlife do during natural disasters?,” WolfCenter.org, 03/17/21
Source: “The animals that detect disasters,” BBC.com, 02/02/22
Source: “The attraction of insects to forest fires,” Frames.gov, 1972
Source: “Formica obscuripes,” AntWiki.org, undated
Image by Shan Sheehan/CC BY-ND 2.0

Do Animals Know What to Do?

Where does drive-reduction theory come in, when animals experience a drive to preserve themselves from burning alive? What happens in a fire? Does the flight reflex inevitably kick in, or do animals react with displacement behaviors? Because if, under those very threatening circumstances, they do not resort to displacement behaviors, wouldn’t that put a rather large hole in the entire concept?

As it turns out, wild animals have a whole array of responses to wildfire. Some run in panic, others move calmly away, some swim along rivers, and others are even attracted toward the devastation. Some species, who have been adapting for eons, act in ways that seem counter-intuitive to humans, unless we take the trouble to understand. Jara Gutiérrez and Francisco Javier de Miguel report:

Some animals […] have advantageous evolutionary olfactory, visual, chemical, or audible fire detection mechanisms. Even in deep torpor, some of them can detect fire or smoke and then display active escape or refuge seeking behaviors [B]ehavioral responses may be detrimental to individuals, as happens with some animals climbing trees or animals not burrowing deep enough…

In other words, they are not just hanging around, dithering indecisively, picking at grass or faking sleep, or doing other displacement behaviors. Of course, not every effort is redemptive. When a creature acts on what could or should have been a solid instinct, its fatal failure is not due to a maladaptive response, but to individual bad luck.

E. V. Komarek, Sr., who seems to have known more about this subject than anyone, learned that some animals do not have an innate fear of fire, but deal with it matter-of-factly. Cotton rats, for example, spread the alarm to their neighbors and efficiently get to work evacuating the young from their nests to safer areas, without regard for the approaching flames. Sometimes they hide in ant mounds. Komarek wrote:

Somehow, mammals have the ability to sense the fire, smoke, and the direction it travels. These animals do not panic and flee ahead of a wind driven fire, but they usually escape along the sides or flanks… My associates and I have observed Virginia deer quietly watching a fire at night while slowly moving away from the flames… Horses, cattle and dogs have been seen warming themselves quite near moving flames.

King vultures will flock to a fire like kids to a free concert, knowing they will reap a bounteous feast of roasted reptiles. But not of cottontail rabbits! In his vast experience, this scientist never found a cottontail rabbit that had been damaged in a fire, including juveniles who normally stayed in the nests. A lot of snakes apparently know exactly what to do:

There are 500,000 or more acres in the Thomasville, Georgia-Tallahassee, Florida hunting lands that are burned over annually. The southern diamondback rattlesnake inhabits these areas. The annual burning does not seem to have reduced the number of such snakes, and I have not seen a rattlesnake killed by fire, and only rarely heard of such an occurrence… I have only found two water moccasins killed by fire on the many acres of marsh I have investigated.

(To be continued)

Your responses and feedback are welcome!

Source: “Challenges posed by fires to wild animals and how to help,” Animal-Ethics.org, 2020
Source: “Fire and Animal Behavior,” TallTimbers.org, 1969
Image by Iforce/Public Domain

Drive-Reduction Theory Dissected

Drive reduction theory is a confusing area, just like any other explanatory notion where, in actual experiential life, the exceptions seem to outnumber the rule. Here is a pretty basic definition:

The purpose of drive reduction is to conserve internal stability (homeostasis). The ability of a system or living organism to adjust its internal environment to maintain a stable equilibrium; such as the ability of warm-blooded animals to maintain a constant temperature. Drive-Reduction Theory works well for simple motivations…

But does it? Does the drive-reduction theory work well for simple motivations? For animals, sure. For humans, not so much. Take the example cited here, maintaining a constant temperature. An animal that is uncomfortably warm in the sun will go into the shade. An animal threatened by fire will, if possible, get into some water.

But humans do this stuff to themselves on purpose. A human will lie around roasting in the sun, anointing itself with potions to keep its skin from cooking, and purposely build up a very uncomfortable degree of overheated discomfort, the better to enjoy an eventual plunge into a pool.

If at all possible, an animal escaping fire or a predator will run away. If the threat closes the distance and seems about to catch up, the animal’s drive to escape and survive will escalate, and it will run even faster. So far, this makes sense.

People get involved, and it all falls apart

Take the most literal drive: driving. Humans go to great trouble and expense to produce machines that go fast for no other purpose than to go fast. There are of course instances where speed is very important — for instance, when a vehicle needs to achieve escape velocity to leave the earth’s gravitational field. And yet people devote their entire lives to, and often sacrifice their lives on behalf of, vehicles that go faster than the average person ever has a need for. This drive is not based on a physiological need, or on any purpose related to the survival of the individual or the species. It is arbitrary and, for practical purposes, useless.

As previously mentioned, the smell of fresh-baked bread stimulates both the starved and the well-fed, and arouses their drive to eat. Huge, very lucrative industries are based on the arbitrary creation of drives and incentives that are artificially cultivated and have nothing to do with the good of the species, and in fact, kill members of the species in huge numbers.

Needs?

The source says, “Drive theory is based on the principle that organisms are born with certain physiological needs…” Coca leaves grow in some geographical areas, and while workers there discovered how to chew the leaves to take the edge off hunger and exhaustion, the chemical was never a physiological need. Neither any individual nor the human species would have died without it.

Before cocaine started being refined, no human had ever been born with an innate need to snort refined cocaine. We synthesize drugs that both create and satisfy our drive to consume them. Somewhat like potato chips, addictive drugs exemplify an industry devoted to the satisfaction of a perceived and deeply felt need that would not have even existed without the stimulus provided by the industry itself.

Your responses and feedback are welcome!

Source: “Drive-Reduction,” InStructure.com, undated
Image by Jake Archibald/CC BY 2.0

What Is Drive-Reduction Theory?

It seems that in its simplest form, drive-reduction theory has to do with physiological (bodily) needs. One source gives this definition:

If we were to get too hot (need to be cool), we would seek shade (the drive).

There seems to be something not quite accurate about this. Actually, the physiological drive, the bodily need, is to be cool. This could be accomplished in several ways, of which seeking shade is only one possibility. Getting into the water would be another. For a human, opening the refrigerator door and standing in front of it would be another. So, seeking shade is not the drive. Seeking coolness is the drive.

In truth, when the sun is too hot, it is the drive to cool off that causes a move into the shade of a tree. Or the dive into the water. Or the erection of an umbrella. But the umbrella is not the drive. The refrigerator is not the drive. When a basic definition inspires such niggling doubts, where does the hopeful student go from there? The same page attempts to elucidate, but only sows more confusion:

Understand — “physiological need” means something without which you, or the species will die. So it’s a pretty short list — food, water, temperature regulation, sleep, air, and sex.

The situation quickly becomes more complicated. The author says,

A starving person feels driven to eat at the smell of baking bread, and the bread itself becomes the incentive.

Here is a problem. When the scent of baking bread hits their noses, non-starving people also feel driven to eat. It is a stimulus that affects even someone who has very recently finished a satisfying meal. The gigantic advertising industry is the same. Someone who has not eaten in two days might see a billboard that depicts an ice cream sandwich, and feel hunger pangs. But a very well-fed person, who is not the least bit calorie-deficient, can see that same billboard and also feel a tremendously strong desire to eat that ice cream sandwich.

An incidental question

As soon as psychology enters the equation, wouldn’t any research done on animals, to establish any of the basics, have to be eliminated? Because animal psychology is a very limited area, at best. Sure, a dog that has been beaten will show what looks very much like a psychological reaction — it will cower at the sight of a stick. And pets definitely have the psychological savvy to guilt-trip or charm their humans into handing out extra treats.

But human psychology is a vast and varied field, where so much is going on at every moment, it makes some of these notions look pathetically simplistic. Another sentence from the same page says,

Drive-reduction theory states that when a physiological needs arises, so does a psychological drive to reduce the need.

But humans are unique in a propensity to go out of their way to cultivate the existence of drives, and the advertising that sells french fries and sugar-sweetened beverages is but one example.

Recreational drug use is another place where feeble theories go to die. No one can dispute that an addict’s desire for heroin is a colossally powerful drive. But (except in the case of a patient suffering severe pain) there is nothing physiologically, on a body level, natural about a craving for opiates. It is an intentionally created and cultivated drive, caused not by nature, but by invitation, by purposeful action on the part of the human who seeks it. So it hardly deserves to be discussed in the same breath as, for instance, the physiological need to give birth when the baby is ready to come out.

Your responses and feedback are welcome!

Source: “Drive-Reduction,” Instructure.com, undated
Image by Roland Tanglao/CC BY 2.0

FAQs and Media Requests: Click here…

Profiles: Kids Struggling with Weight

Profiles: Kids Struggling with Obesity top bottom

The Book

OVERWEIGHT: What Kids Say explores the obesity problem from the often-overlooked perspective of children struggling with being overweight.

About Dr. Robert A. Pretlow

Dr. Robert A. Pretlow is a pediatrician and childhood obesity specialist. He has been researching and spreading awareness on the childhood obesity epidemic in the US for more than a decade.
You can contact Dr. Pretlow at:

Presentations

Dr. Pretlow’s invited presentation at the American Society of Animal Science 2020 Conference
What’s Causing Obesity in Companion Animals and What Can We Do About It

Dr. Pretlow’s invited presentation at the World Obesity Federation 2019 Conference:
Food/Eating Addiction and the Displacement Mechanism

Dr. Pretlow’s Multi-Center Clinical Trial Kick-off Speech 2018:
Obesity: Tackling the Root Cause

Dr. Pretlow’s 2017 Workshop on
Treatment of Obesity Using the Addiction Model

Dr. Pretlow’s invited presentation for
TEC and UNC 2016

Dr. Pretlow’s invited presentation at the 2015 Obesity Summit in London, UK.

Dr. Pretlow’s invited keynote at the 2014 European Childhood Obesity Group Congress in Salzburg, Austria.

Dr. Pretlow’s presentation at the 2013 European Congress on Obesity in Liverpool, UK.

Dr. Pretlow’s presentation at the 2011 International Conference on Childhood Obesity in Lisbon, Portugal.

Dr. Pretlow’s presentation at the 2010 Uniting Against Childhood Obesity Conference in Houston, TX.

Food & Health Resources